aaa | Learning Orientation ResearchIndividual Differences in Learning |
aaj |
Home
Lee Cronbach's and Richard Snow's Important Lessons from the Past: Historical Perspective.
A.
In the fifties, Cronbach (1957) challenged the field to find "for each
individual the treatment to which he can most easily adapt." He suggested that consideration of cognitive treatments and individual together would determine the best payoff because we "can expect some attributes of person to have strong interactions with treatment variables.
These attributes have far greater practical importance than the attributes which have little or no interaction."
"In dividing pupils between college preparatory and non-college studies,
for example, a general intelligence test is probably the wrong thing
to use.
This test being general, predicts
success in all subjects, therefore tends to have little interaction
with treatment, and if so is not the best guide to differential treatment."
"We require a measure of aptitude that remains to be discovered.
Ultimately we should design treatments, not to fit the average person,
but to fit groups of students with particular aptitude patterns.
Conversely, we should seek out the aptitudes which correspond to
(interact with) modifiable aspects of the treatment." For example,
how do we sequence treatments differently to
fit groups of students appropriately.
B.
Between 1960 and 1970 Cronbach (1975) and others "searched fruitlessly for interactions of abilities." They were looking for "aptitudes" (characteristics that affects responses to the treatment) that explained how to instruct students one way and not another,
i.e., evidence that showed regression slopes that differed from treatment to treatment.
C.
In the seventies, Cronbach (1975) still advocated that a closer scrutiny of cognitive processes would be a profitable next phase of work on Aptitude Treatment Interactions (ATIs). He highlighted research that related success to the Ai (Achievement via Independence) and Ac (Achievement via Conformance) scores of Gough's California Personality Inventory. The evidence continued to show that the learning outcomes were better when the instructor's presentation adapted to the student's aptitude and personality (1977). For example, the "constructively motivated student who seeks challenges and takes responsibility is at his best when an instructor challenges him and then leaves him to pursue his own thoughts projects."
Conversely, ATI reseach describes how some students with low ability perform better
in highly structured treatments (e.g., high level of external control, well-defined sequences, and structured components).
However, similar treatments hinder those with high abilities and preferences for less structured treatments.
In his article, Cronbach (1975) continued to emphasize the important
relationship between cognitive aptitudes and treatment interactions.
Nevertheless, he states that "Snow and I have been thwarted by the
inconsistent findings coming from roughly similar inquiries. Successive
studies employing the same treatment variable find different
outcome-on-aptitude slopes." He surmised that the inconsistency came from
unidentified interactions. Finally, Snow and Cronbach (1977) concluded that
"an understanding of cognitive abilities considered alone would not be
sufficient" to explain learning, individual differences in learning, and
aptitude treatment interactions.
D.
In the early eighties, the cognitive process analysis of aptitudes
processes continued with variations. Snow (1980) described the ATI
investigation as process-oriented research on individual differences in
learning and cognition. Although they were looking for a "whole-person view"
of learning, he believed that it was primarily the cognitive processes that
should be considered in the design and development of adaptive instructional
systems. Eventually the new "aptitudes" evolved into cognitive styles
(learning styles) to represent the predominant modes of information
processing (i.e., preferred learning sets to the acquisition, retention, and
retrieval of new knowledge). ATI critics argued that student performance was
too dynamic to be supported by the permanence and pervasiveness of primarily
cognitive ATI and that students, e.g., without learner control, would become
system dependent on prescribed solutions.
E.
In the late eighties, Snow (1987) described how in cognitive psychology
conation as a learning factor has been "demoted" and "since it seems not
really to be a separable function," it is merged with affection. Together
these factors are viewed as "mere associates or products of cognition" and
then ignored. He warned that individual difference constructs or "aptitude
complexes" needed greater consideration of the joint functioning between
cognitive, conative, and affective processes. Snow was in search of an
information processing model of cognition that would include (still as a
secondary consideration) possible cognitive-conative-affective intersections.
He was looking for a way to fit realistic "aspects of mental life, such as
mood, emotion, impulse, desire, volition, purposive striving" into
instructional models. According to Snow (1989), the best instruction
involves treatments that differ in structure and completeness and high or low
general ability measures. Highly structured treatments (e.g., high external
control, explicit sequences and components) seem to help students with low
ability but hinder those with high abilities (relative to low structure
treatments).
F.
Cronbach's and Snow's research set the stage for the learning orientation
research. The learning orientation research attempts to reveal the dominant
power of emotions and intentions on guiding and managing cognitive processes
(no longer demoted to a secondary role). It is in understanding the
structure and nature of the complex relationships between learning
orientations and interactions that we can return to Cronbach's original
hypothesis that we should find "for each individual the treatment to which he
can most easily adapt." And, "ultimately we should design treatments, not to
fit the average person, but to fit groups of students with particular
aptitude patterns. Conversely, we should seek out the aptitudes which
correspond to (interact with) modifiable aspects of the treatment."
As can be expected the new lines of research, especially the
neurobiology of learning and memory research, will continue to reopen the
old questions, gain from the research accomplished in the past, and pose
exciting new questions for the future. As we look forward to new issues
highlighting the importance of emotional and intentional states on cognitive
processing, waiting in the wings to be discovered are the treatments that
lead toward more successful learning and performance. And perhaps, as some
may have already predicted in the past the hegemony of cognition over intent
and affect is coming to an end.
Sources
Cronbach, L. (1975). Beyond the Two Disciplines of Scientific Psychology.
"American Psychologist," 116-127.
|